|
|
|
Dr. George Gill (and Jaime Stuart)
|
Does Race Exist? A proponent's perspective
by George W. Gill
Slightly over half of all biological/physical anthropologists
today believe in the traditional view that human races are
biologically valid and real. Furthermore, they tend to see
nothing wrong in defining and naming the different populations
of Homo sapiens. The other half of the biological
anthropology community believes either that the traditional
racial categories for humankind are arbitrary and meaningless,
or that at a minimum there are better ways to look at human
variation than through the "racial lens."
Are there differences in the research concentrations of these
two groups of experts? Yes, most decidedly there are. As
pointed out in a recent 2000 edition of a popular physical
anthropology textbook, forensic anthropologists (those who do
skeletal identification for law-enforcement agencies) are
overwhelmingly in support of the idea of the basic biological
reality of human races, and yet those who work with
blood-group data, for instance, tend to reject the biological
reality of racial categories.
Where does George Gill stand in the "great race
debate?" Read on.
|
|
I happen to be one of those very few forensic physical
anthropologists who actually does research on the particular
traits used today in forensic racial identification (i.e.,
"assessing ancestry," as it is generally termed today). Partly
this is because for more than a decade now U.S. national and
regional forensic anthropology organizations have deemed it
necessary to quantitatively test both traditional and new
methods for accuracy in legal cases. I volunteered for this
task of testing methods and developing new methods in the late
1980s. What have I found? Where do I now stand in the "great
race debate?" Can I see truth on one side or the
other—or on both sides—in this argument?
Findings
First, I have found that forensic anthropologists attain a
high degree of accuracy in determining geographic racial
affinities (white, black, American Indian, etc.) by utilizing
both new and traditional methods of bone analysis. Many
well-conducted studies were reported in the late 1980s and
1990s that test methods objectively for percentage of correct
placement. Numerous individual methods involving midfacial
measurements, femur traits, and so on are over 80 percent
accurate alone, and in combination produce very high levels of
accuracy. No forensic anthropologist would make a racial
assessment based upon just one of these methods, but in
combination they can make very reliable assessments, just as
in determining sex or age. In other words, multiple criteria
are the key to success in all of these determinations.
|
While he doesn't believe in socially stipulated "age"
categories, Gill says, he can "age" skeletions with
great accuracy.
|
I have a respected colleague, the skeletal biologist C. Loring
Brace, who is as skilled as any of the leading forensic
anthropologists at assessing ancestry from bones, yet he does
not subscribe to the concept of race. [Read
Brace's position on
the concept of race.] Neither does Norman Sauer, a
board-certified forensic anthropologist. My students ask, "How
can this be? They can identify skeletons as to racial origins
but do not believe in race!" My answer is that we can often
function within systems that we do not believe in.
As a middle-aged male, for example, I am not so sure that I
believe any longer in the chronological "age" categories that
many of my colleagues in skeletal biology use. Certainly parts
of the skeletons of some 45-year-old people look older than
corresponding portions of the skeletons of some 55-year-olds.
If, however, law enforcement calls upon me to provide "age" on
a skeleton, I can provide an answer that will be proven
sufficiently accurate should the decedent eventually be
identified. I may not believe in society's "age" categories,
but I can be very effective at "aging" skeletons. The next
question, of course, is how "real" is age biologically? My
answer is that if one can use biological criteria to assess
age with reasonable accuracy, then age has some basis in
biological reality even if the particular "social construct"
that defines its limits might be imperfect. I find this true
not only for age and stature estimations but for sex and race
identification.
"I am more accurate at assessing race from
skeletal remains that from looking at living people
standing before me," Gill says.
|
|
The "reality of race" therefore depends more on the definition
of reality than on the definition of race. If we choose to
accept the system of racial taxonomy that physical
anthropologists have traditionally established—major
races: black, white, etc.—then one can classify human
skeletons within it just as well as one can living humans. The
bony traits of the nose, mouth, femur, and cranium are just as
revealing to a good osteologist as skin color, hair form, nose
form, and lips to the perceptive observer of living humanity.
I have been able to prove to myself over the years, in actual
legal cases, that I am
more accurate at assessing race from skeletal remains
than from looking at living people standing before me. So
those of us in forensic anthropology know that the skeleton
reflects race, whether "real" or not, just as well if not
better than superficial soft tissue does. The idea that race
is "only skin deep" is simply not true, as any experienced
forensic anthropologist will affirm.
Position on race
Where I stand today in the "great race debate" after a decade
and a half of pertinent skeletal research is clearly more on
the side of the reality of race than on the "race denial"
side. Yet I do see why many other physical anthropologists are
able to ignore or deny the race concept. Blood-factor
analysis, for instance, shows many traits that cut across
racial boundaries in a purely
clinal fashion with very few if any "breaks" along
racial boundaries. (A cline is a gradient of change, such as
from people with a high frequency of blue eyes, as in
Scandinavia, to people with a high frequency of brown eyes, as
in Africa.)
|
"Clines" represent gradients of change, such as that
between areas where most people have blue eyes and
areas in which brown eyes predominate.
|
Morphological characteristics, however, like skin color, hair
form, bone traits, eyes, and lips tend to follow geographic
boundaries coinciding often with climatic zones. This is not
surprising since the selective forces of climate are probably
the primary forces of nature that have shaped human races with
regard not only to skin color and hair form but also the
underlying bony structures of the nose, cheekbones, etc. (For
example, more prominent noses humidify air better.) As far as
we know, blood-factor frequencies are
not shaped by these same climatic factors.
So, serologists who work largely with blood factors will tend
to see human variation as clinal and races as not a valid
construct, while skeletal biologists, particularly forensic
anthropologists, will see races as biologically real. The
common person on the street who sees only a person's skin
color, hair form, and face shape will also tend to see races
as biologically real. They are not incorrect. Their
perspective is just different from that of the serologist.
So, yes, I see truth on both sides of the race argument.
Those who believe that the concept of race is valid do not
discredit the notion of clines, however. Yet those with the
clinal perspective who believe that races are not real do try
to discredit the evidence of skeletal biology. Why this bias
from the "race denial" faction? This bias seems to stem
largely from socio-political motivation and not science at
all. For the time being at least, the people in "race denial"
are in "reality denial" as well. Their motivation (a positive
one) is that they have come to believe that the race concept
is socially dangerous. In other words, they have convinced
themselves that race promotes racism. Therefore, they have
pushed the politically correct agenda that human races are not
biologically real, no matter what the evidence.
Consequently, at the beginning of the 21st
century, even as a majority of biological anthropologists
favor the reality of the race perspective, not one
introductory textbook of physical anthropology even presents
that perspective as a possibility. In a case as flagrant as
this, we are not dealing with science but rather with blatant,
politically motivated censorship. But, you may ask, are the
politically correct actually correct? Is there a relationship
between thinking about race and racism?
Does discussing the concept of race promote racism?
|
|
Race and racism
Does discussing human variation in a framework of racial
biology promote or reduce racism? This is an important
question, but one that does not have a simple answer. Most
social scientists over the past decade have convinced
themselves that it runs the risk of promoting racism in
certain quarters. Anthropologists of the 1950s, 1960s, and
early 1970s, on the other hand, believed that they were
combating racism by openly discussing race and by teaching
courses on human races and racism. Which approach has worked
best? What do the intellectuals among racial minorities
believe? How do students react and respond?
Three years ago, I served on a NOVA-sponsored panel in New
York, in which panelists debated the topic "Is There Such a
Thing as Race?" Six of us sat on the panel, three proponents
of the race concept and three antagonists. All had authored
books or papers on race. Loring Brace and I were the two
anthropologists "facing off" in the debate. The ethnic
composition of the panel was three white and three black
scholars. As our conversations developed, I was struck by how
similar many of my concerns regarding racism were to those of
my two black teammates. Although recognizing that embracing
the race concept can have risks attached, we were (and are)
more fearful of the form of racism likely to emerge if race is
denied and dialogue about it lessened. We fear that the social
taboo about the subject of race has served to suppress open
discussion about a very important subject in need of
dispassionate debate. One of my teammates, an
affirmative-action lawyer, is afraid that a denial that races
exist also serves to encourage a denial that racism exists. He
asks, "How can we combat racism if no one is willing to talk
about race?"
|
"How can we combat racism," asks an
affirmative-action lawyer, "if no one is willing to
talk about race?"
|
Who will benefit?
In my experience, minority students almost invariably have
been the strongest supporters of a "racial perspective" on
human variation in the classroom. The first-ever black student
in my human variation class several years ago came to me at
the end of the course and said, "Dr. Gill, I really want to
thank you for changing my life with this course." He went on
to explain that, "My whole life I have wondered about why I am
black, and if that is good or bad. Now I know the reasons why
I am the way I am and that these traits are useful and good."
A human-variation course with another perspective would
probably have accomplished the same for this student if he had
ever noticed it. The truth is, innocuous contemporary
human-variation classes with their politically correct titles
and course descriptions do not attract the attention of
minorities or those other students who could most benefit.
Furthermore, the politically correct "race denial" perspective
in society as a whole suppresses dialogue, allowing ignorance
to replace knowledge and suspicion to replace familiarity.
This encourages ethnocentrism and racism more than it
discourages it.
Dr. George W. Gill is a professor of anthropology at the
University of Wyoming. He also serves as the forensic
anthropologist for Wyoming law-enforcement agencies and the
Wyoming State Crime Laboratory.
Does Race Exist? |
Meet Kennewick Man
Claims for the Remains
|
The Dating Game
| Resources
Transcript
| Site Map |
Mystery of the First Americans Home
Editor's Picks
|
Previous Sites
|
Join Us/E-mail
|
TV/Web Schedule
About NOVA |
Teachers |
Site Map |
Shop |
Jobs |
Search |
To print
PBS Online |
NOVA Online |
WGBH
©
| Updated November 2000
|
|
|